[Date Prev][Date Next][Thread Prev][Thread Next][Date Index][Thread Index]
VMs: O.T.: The Indus Script--Write or Wrong? (Science)
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/306/5704/2026?etoc
Science, Vol 306, Issue 5704, 2026-2029 , 17 December 2004
ARCHAEOLOGY:
The Indus Script--Write or Wrong?
Andrew Lawler
For 130 years scholars have struggled to decipher the Indus script. Now, in a
proposal with broad academic and political implications, a brash outsider
claims that such efforts are doomed to failure because the Indus symbols are not
writing
Academic prizes typically are designed to confer prestige. But the latest
proposed award, a $10,000 check for finding a lengthy inscription from the
ancient Indus civilization, is intended to goad rather than honor. The controversial
scholar who announced the prize last month cheekily predicts that he will
never have to pay up. Going against a century of scholarship, he and a growing
number of linguists and archaeologists assert that the Indus people--unlike
their Egyptian and Mesopotamian contemporaries 4000 years ago--could not write.
That claim is part of a bitter clash among academics, as well as between
Western scientists and Indian nationalists, over the nature of the Indus society,
a clash that has led to shouting matches and death threats. But the
provocative proposal, summed up in a paper published online last week, is winning
adherents within the small community of Indus scholars who say it is time to rethink
an enigmatic society that spanned a vast area in today's Pakistan, India, and
Afghanistan--the largest civilization of its day.
The Indus civilization has intrigued and puzzled researchers for more than
130 years, with their sophisticated sewers, huge numbers of wells, and a notable
lack of monumental architecture or other signs of an elite class (see sidebar
on p. 2027). Most intriguing of all is the mysterious system of symbols, left
on small tablets, pots, and stamp seals. But without translations into a
known script--the "Rosetta stones" that led to the decipherment of Egyptian
hieroglyphics and Sumerian cuneiform in the 19th century--hundreds of attempts to
understand the symbols have so far failed. And what language the system might
have expressed--such as a Dravidian language similar to tongues of today's
southern India, or a Vedic language of northern India--is also a hot topic. This is
no dry discussion: Powerful Indian nationalists of the Hindutva movement see
the Indus civilization as the direct ancestor to Hindu tradition and Vedic
culture.
[Searching for script. Richard Meadow excavates at Harappa.
CREDITS: COURTESY OF THE HARAPPA ARCHAEOLOGICAL RESEARCH PROJECT/PHOTOS
BY R. H. MEADOW]
Now academic outsider Steve Farmer (see sidebar on p. 2028) and two
established Indus scholars argue that the signs are not writing at all but rather a
collection of religious-political symbols that held together a diverse and
multilingual society. The brevity of most inscriptions, the relative frequencies of
symbols, and the lack of archaeological evidence of a manuscript tradition add
up to a sign system that does not encode language, argue historian Farmer and
his co-authors, Harvard University linguist Michael Witzel and computational
theorist Richard Sproat of the University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.
Instead, they say the signs may have more in common with European medieval
heraldry, the Christian cross, or a bevy of magical symbols used by prehistoric
peoples.
This idea has profound implications for how the Indus civilization lived and
died. Instead of the monolithic, peaceful, and centralized empire envisioned
by some scholars, the authors say that the new view points to a giant
multilingual society in which a system of religious-political signs provided cohesion.
Their thesis has bitterly divided the field of Indus studies, made up of a
small and close-knit bunch dominated by Americans. Some respected archaeologists
and linguists flatly reject it. "I categorically disagree that the script
does not reflect a language," says archaeologist J. Mark Kenoyer of the
University of Wisconsin, Madison, who co-directs a dig at the key site of Harappa in
Pakistan. "What the heck were they doing if not encoding language?" Asko
Parpola, a linguist at Finland's University of Helsinki who has worked for decades to
decipher the signs, says. "There is no chance it is not a script; this is a
fully formed system. It was a phonetic script." Linguist Gregory Possehl of the
University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia says that it is not possible to
"prove" the script cannot be deciphered. All three argue that Farmer's thesis is
a pessimistic and defeatist approach to a challenging problem. Meanwhile, the
very idea that the Indus civilization was not literate is deeply offensive to
many Indian nationalists.
Yet since a 2002 meeting at Harvard University at which Farmer laid out a
detailed theory--and was greeted with shouts of derision--he has attracted
important converts, including his co-authors. A growing cadre of scholars back the
authors' approach as a fresh way to look at a vexing problem and an opportunity
to shed new light on many of the mysteries that haunt Indus research. Harvard
anthropologist Richard Meadow, who with Kenoyer directs the Harappa project,
calls the paper "an extremely valuable contribution" that could cut the
Gordian knot bedeviling the field. Sanskrit and South Asian linguist Witzel says he
was shocked when he first heard Farmer's contention in 2001. "I thought I
could read a few of the signs," Witzel recalls. "So I was very skeptical." Now he
is throwing his scholarly weight behind the new thesis, as a co-author of the
paper and also editor of the Electronic Journal of Vedic Studies, an online
journal aimed at rapid publication, which published the paper. Addsarchaeologist
Steven Weber of Washington State University in Vancouver: "Sometimes it takes
someone from the outside to ask the really basic questions." Weber, who is
now collaborating with Farmer, adds that "the burden of proof now has to be on
the people who say it is writing."
Seeking the Write Stuff
Since the 1870s, archaeologists have uncovered more than 4000 Indus
inscriptions on a variety of media. Rudimentary signs appear around 3200 B.C.E.--the
same era in which hieroglyphics and cuneiform began to appear in Egypt and Iraq.
By 2800 B.C.E., the signs become more durable, continuing in use in later
periods; the greatest diversity starts to appear around 2400 B.C.E. Some signs
are highly abstract, whereas others seem to have obvious pictographic qualities,
such as one that looks like a fish and another that resembles a jar. Both are
used frequently; the jar sign accounts for one in 10 symbols, says Possehl.
As in Mesopotamia, the signs typically appear on small tablets made of clay as
well as on stamp seals. The seals often are accompanied by images of animals
and plants, both real and mythical.
The signs start to diminish around 1900 B.C.E. and vanish entirely by 1700
B.C.E., when the Indus culture disappears. Oddly, the inscriptions are almost
all found in trash dumps rather than in graves or in primary contexts such as
the floor of a home. "They were thrown away like expired credit cards," says
Meadow.
No one had ever seriously questioned whether the signs are a form of writing.
But scholars hotly debate whether the system is phonetic like English or
Greek or logosyllabic--using a combination of symbols that encode both sound and
concepts--like cuneiform or hieroglyphics. Even the number of signs is
controversial. Archaeologist and linguist S. R. Rao of India's University of Goa has
proposed a sign list of only 20, but Harvard graduate student Bryan Wells is
compiling a revised list now numbering 700; most estimates hover in the 400
range.
Farmer and colleagues reanalyzed the signs, drawing on published data from
many sites and unpublished data from the Harappa project provided by Meadow.
They found that the average Indus inscription, out of a total of 4000 to 5000 in
a 1977 compilation, has 4.6 signs. The longest known inscription contains 17
signs, and fewer than 1% are as long as 10 symbols. The authors argued that
such short "texts" are unprecedented for actual writing. Although many scholars
assert that longer inscriptions may have been made on perishable materials, the
authors note that there is no archaeological evidence of the imperishable
paraphernalia that typically accompanies literate culture, such as inkpots, rock
inscriptions, or papermaking devices.
Farmer and colleagues also take apart a long-held assumption that the
frequent repetition of a small number of Indus signs is evidence of a script encoding
language. About 12% of an average English text, for example, consists of the
letter "E," often used repeatedly in a single sentence to express a certain
sound. In contrast, the paper notes that very few Indus symbols are repeated
within individual inscriptions, implying that the signs do not encode sounds.
Further, the authors note that many Indus symbols are incredibly rare. Half
of the symbols appear only once, based on Wells's catalog; three-quarters of
the signs appear five times or fewer. According to the 1977 compilation put
together by Iravatham Mahadevan, an Indian linguist now retired in Chennai, India,
more than one-fourth of all signs appear only once, and more than half show
up five times or fewer. Rarely used signs likely would not encode sound, says
Farmer. It is as if many symbols "were invented on the fly, only to be
abandoned after being used once or a handful of times," he, Witzel, and Sproat write.
[Short and sweet. Most Indus inscriptions are short.
CREDIT: COURTESY OF THE HARAPPA ARCHAEOLOGICAL RESEARCH PROJECT/PHOTOS BY
R. H. MEADOW]
Farmer believes that the symbols have nonlinguistic meaning. He speculates
that the signs may have been considered magical--as the Christian cross can
be--and indicated individuals or clans, cities or professions, or gods. He and his
colleagues compare the Indus script to inscriptions found in prehistoric
southeastern Europe around 4000 B.C.E., where the Vina culture produced an array
of symbols often displayed in a linear form, including a handful used
frequently.
But these conclusions are not accepted by key archaeologists and linguists
who have spent their careers digging at Harappa or trying to decipher the
symbols. "Regularities in the frequency and distribution of signs are possible only
in a linguistic script," says Mahadevan. Wells is more blunt. "He is utterly
wrong," he says of Farmer. "There is something you recognize as an epigrapher
immediately, such as long linear patterns."
As to the brevity of inscriptions, Wells says averages can be misleading. The
longer Indus inscriptions, he says, can't be explained as magical symbols.
Vina symbols, for example, rarely are grouped in numbers greater than five. "And
you don't get repetitive ordering" as with Indus signs, he adds. "The Indus
script is a highly patterned, highly ordered system with a syntax--it just
looks too much like writing." Wells also says that a mere 30 signs are used only
once, rather than the 1000 Farmer postulates, because many of the "singletons"
transform into compound signs used repeatedly.
Parpola agrees that the pattern of symbols argues for an organized script.
"There are a limited number of standardized signs, some repeated hundreds of
times--with the same shape, recurring combinations, and regular lines," he says.
But Wells and Parpola, like most linguists in the field, agree on little
beyond their opposition to Farmer. Wells rejects Parpola's method of deciphering
the signs, and Parpola dismisses Wells's contention that there are significant
differences between the signs of upper and lower Indus.
Wells and some other scholars believe that the attraction of Farmer's idea
has less to do with science than with the long history of decipherment failures.
"Some have turned to this idea that it is not writing out of frustration," he
says.
[Sign or script? Farmer says Indus seals (left), like Vina signs (right) are
not writing.
CREDIT: COURTESY OF THE HARAPPA ARCHAEOLOGICAL RESEARCH PROJECT/PHOTOS BY
R. H. MEADOW]
But many others are convinced that Farmer, Witzel, and Sproat have found a
way to move away from sterile discussions of decipherment, and they find few
flaws in their arguments. "They have settled the issue for me," says George
Thompson, a Sanskrit scholar at Montserrat College of Art in Beverly,
Massachusetts. "We have the work of a comparative historian, a computational linguist, and
a Vedicist," he adds. "Together they have changed the landscape regarding the
whole question." In a forthcoming book on South Asian linguistic archaeology,
Frank Southworth of the University of Pennsylvania calls the paper an
"unexpected solution" to the old troubles with decipherment.
Meanwhile, Farmer is injecting a bit of fun into the melee. "Find us just one
inscription with 50 symbols on it, in repeating symbols in the kinds of
quasi-random patterns associated with true scripts, and we'll consider our model
falsified," he wrote on a listserve devoted to the Indus. And he is putting his
money--or, rather, that of a donor he won't reveal--where his mouth is,
promising the winner $10,000. The orthodox dismiss the prize as grandstanding,
whereas Farmer boasts that "no one is ever going to collect that money."
Retrenching
Each side clearly has far to go to convince its opponents. "I'm not sure the
case is strong enough on either side," says linguist Hans Hock of the
University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. "Let each side of the controversy make their
case."
Yet there already is a retreat from earlier claims that the Indus symbols
represent a full-blown writing system and that they encoded speech. Many scholars
such as Possehl now acknowledge that the signs likely are dominated by names
of places, people, clans, plants, and gods rather than by the narratives found
in ancient Sumer or Egypt. They say the script may be more similar to the
first stages of writing in those lands. Harvard archaeologist Carl
Lamberg-Karlovsky says the meanings of the Indus signs likely are "impenetrable and
imponderable" and adds that whether or not the signs are considered writing, they
clearly are a form of communication--and that is what really counts. Recent
research in Central and South America has highlighted how complex societies
prospered without traditional writing, such as the knotted strings or khipu of the
vast Incan empire (Science, 2 July, p. 30).
[Literacy promoter. J. Mark Kenoyer, on the dig at Harappa, thinks Indus
signs are script.
CREDIT: COURTESY OF THE HARAPPA ARCHAEOLOGICAL RESEARCH PROJECT/PHOTOS BY
R. H. MEADOW]
Farmer adds that a society does not need to be literate to be complex. "A
big, urban civilization can be held together without writing," he says. He and
his co-authors suggest that the Indus likely had many tongues and was a rich mix
of ethnicities like India today. Wells has found marked differences between
signs in the upper and lower Indus River regions, backing up the theory of a
more diverse society. But some, such as D. P. Agrawal, an independent
archaeologist based in Almora, India, doubt that a civilization spread over more than 1
million square kilometers, and with uniform weights, measures, and developed
trade, could manage its affairs without a script.
This debate over Indus literacy has political as well as academic
consequences. "This will be seen as an attack on the greatness of Indian
civilization--which would be unfortunate," says Shereen Ratnagar, a retired archaeologist who
taught at Delhi's Nehru University. Tension is already high between some
Western and Indian scholars and Indian nationalists. "Indologists are at war with
the Hindutva polemicists," says statistical linguist Lars Martin Fosse of the
University of Oslo, and the issue of the script "is extremely sensitive."
Farmer says he regularly receives e-mail viruses and death threats from Indian
nationalists who oppose his views.
For decades, Indus researchers have tended to stick with their established
positions, as on the script, a tendency that has kept the field from moving
forward, says one archaeologist who compares the small cadre of Indus scholars to
a "dysfunctional family" with a proclivity for secrecy, ideological positions,
and intolerance. Meadow is among those who argue that it is time to set aside
old ideas, no matter how much time and effort has been invested in them, in
order to push the field forward. "We're here to do science, and it is always
valuable to have new models," he says. Adds Ratnagar: "We must get back to an
open mind." Given the strong emotions swirling around the Indus symbols,
discovering the key to that open mind may prove the hardest code to break.
Volume 306, Number 5704, Issue of 17 Dec 2004, pp. 2026-2029.
Copyright © 2004 by The American Association for the Advancement of Science.
All rights reserved.
...
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/306/5704/2028?etoc
Science, Vol 306, Issue 5704, 2028 , 17 December 2004
ARCHAEOLOGY:
Outsider Revels in Breaking Academic Taboos
Andrew Lawler
Steve Farmer describes himself as "the ultimate collaborationist," but he has
a way of making enemies. When he showed up at a 2002 Harvard University
gathering to propose that the Indus script is no script at all, participants recall
that his ideas were greeted with shouts of derision. And his positions on the
role of the Indus civilization in Indian history have earned him a place in
the demonology of Indian nationalists.
Yet despite what many call an abrasive personality, this former street kid
from Chicago, who lacks a high school diploma, has shaken up the closed field of
Indus studies (see main text). "It is healthy the way this is turning things
upside down," says archaeologist Steven Weber of Washington State University
in Vancouver.
Farmer's linguistic ability got him off the streets when he joined the Army
in the 1960s. After learning Russian at the military's language school in
Monterey, California, he worked for the National Security Agency listening in on
the conversations of Soviet pilots. Then, radicalized by the Vietnam War, he
left the military for academia. After winning a high school equivalency diploma,
he studied history at the University of Maryland, College Park, and earned a
Ph.D. in comparative cultural history at Stanford University in California. He
taught history of science and European history at George Mason University
outside Washington, D.C., and then moved to Louisiana State University in Baton
Rouge as a tenure-track professor. But he says he rejected full-time academic
life to avoid teaching courses he found boring and moved back to California,
where he was on the adjunct faculty at Ohlone College in Fremont until 1997. To
support his scholarly pursuits, Farmer has edited a journal on narcolepsy,
worked on a PGA golf tournament training program, and helped develop a device to
aid people with brain disorders.
In 1999, after putting together a model of cross-cultural frameworks for
premodern history using ancient China as an example, he turned his attention to
India. "I didn't know anything about this stuff," he says. "I was the naïve
outsider too dumb not to recognize the field's taboos." But he was struck by the
brevity of Indus inscriptions and unconvinced by the many efforts to decipher
the symbols. He didn't hesitate to poke fun at Indian nationalists who
attempted their own decipherments and who promulgated theories connecting the Indus to
Hindu culture. "I still get death threats daily," he says. "And I'm careful
about opening packages from India." He also was irritated by what he calls
archaeologists' proclivity to "hoard data."
"He can be abrasive and aggressive, and many in the field find him
presumptuous," says linguist George Thompson of Montserrat College of Art in Beverly,
Massachusetts. At the 2002 Harvard meeting, a few of the academics present
hooted Farmer off the stage. "People were literally screaming," Farmer recalls.
Nonetheless, his arguments ultimately impressed Harvard anthropologist Richard
Meadow, who granted him access to unpublished Harappa data. "Steve stepped in
and did an enormous amount of work" on the Harappa data, says Thompson.
His arrogance makes him hard for some scholars to get along with. "I've
remade the field," he recently boasted. Others resent his methods. "He uses verbose
arguments," says archaeologist J. Mark Kenoyer of the University of
Wisconsin, Madison, co-director of the Harappa dig. "And he's not basing it on
science." Adds linguist Gregory Possehl of the University of Pennsylvania in
Philadelphia, "I don't think his ideas are interesting or viable, and I'm surprised
they have raised interest." At this point, however, that interest is undeniable,
so Indus specialists are making room, albeit reluctantly, for a new member of
their small family. But the intellectually peripatetic Farmer insists he will
not make himself at home: "This is just a chapter in my book."
Volume 306, Number 5704, Issue of 17 Dec 2004, p. 2028.
Copyright © 2004 by The American Association for the Advancement of Science.
All rights reserved.
______________________________________________________________________
To unsubscribe, send mail to majordomo@xxxxxxxxxxx with a body saying:
unsubscribe vms-list